Nothing But or Anything But?
by Marian Stamp Dawkins
source | |
Are you one of those people who dislikes science because of what you
think it does to the "mystery" of the world? Do you think that it
somehow diminishes people to try to "explain" their behaviour or that
it "takes away the beauty of the rainbow" to talk about light rays
passing through water droplets? |
If you are, can I ask you to pause just for a moment and
very briefly allow your mind to entertain the opposite view, perhaps
one that will strike you as utterly preposterous and even repugnant?
You can, if you like, think of yourself as being like the captured
prince in C. S. Lewis's story The Silver Chair-the
one who had to be tied down every evening at six o'clock because, for
an unpleasant few minutes, he had hallucinations and then recovered and
was perfectly normal for the next twenty-four hours. The hallucinations
will not last long, in other words. You are quite safe. Just five
minutes of raging lunacy is all you are in for.
The utterly
preposterous idea is this: Explaining something in a scientific way
does not diminish it. It enhances it. Let me tell you why.
Understanding how things work, even your own brain, has a grandeur and
a glory that no nonscientific explanation can come anywhere near.
I do not, of course, expect you to accept this without question.
But I do ask that you start by thinking of something reasonable and not
scientific at all-like, say, Abraham Lincoln. Ask yourself whether it
enhances or diminishes your view of his achievement when you remember
that he was "nothing but" a country-born, self-taught lawyer. Then ask
yourself which you admire most: someone who starts from an unpromising
background and achieves great things by his own efforts and
personality, or someone who comes from a wealthy and powerful family
and achieves high office because he has an influential father. I would
be surprised if Abraham Lincoln didn't come out of that comparison very
well. Saying he is "nothing but" a backwoodsman doesn't get rid of him
that easily.
Next, try the pyramids.
Does it diminish the achievements of the ancient Egyptians to say that
they built them with "nothing but" the crudest of tools and measuring
instruments? My own sense of awe and admiration for them only went up
when I realised that they moved gigantic blocks of stone with no
wheeled vehicles and that they built the pyramids with perfectly square
bases using "nothing but" lengths of knotted cord
and stakes in the ground. Even the slightest error would have led to
the whole structure being hopelessly out of shape, and yet there they
remain to this day-phenomenal feats of engineering and nearly perfect
shapes. "Nothing but" simple equipment becomes "anything but" a mean
achievement.
Perhaps by now you can see what I am getting at.
If you look at a rainbow and then someone tells you how it comes about,
why should you say that "they have ruined it" by turning it into
"nothing but" a trick of light and water?
Why not say the opposite? Out of the utterly unpromising raw material
of drops of water and the laws of refraction has come something so
beautiful that it spans the sky and inspires poets to write about it.
And if you look at animals and plants
and all the extraordinary structures and behaviour they have, why
reject a scientific explanation of how they got here on the grounds
that it makes them "nothing but" the products of a blind evolutionary
process? You could instead turn and face the full grandeur of the
implications of what that process implies. The bird that builds a nest
and brings food to its young may be "nothing but" the result of evolution by natural selection. But what a result! Birds, just like us, owe their existence to instructions carried on DNA molecules. The scientists in Jurassic Park
who grew dinosaurs out of DNA molecules that were found in preserved
blood were on the right track, even though nobody has yet done this in
practice. DNA molecules do indeed carry the instructions for building
all kinds of bodies-dinosaurs, birds, giant sequoias, and even human
beings.
And the molecules do not stop there. Every breath you
take, every extra second you remain alive depends on hundreds of
chemical reactions all taking place at the right time. Life would be
impossible, for instance, if the body did not have a constant source of
energy-and that energy comes from molecules such as glucose. Glucose
can provide energy, because the three different sorts of atoms that
make it up- carbon, hydrogen, and oxygen-are kept together by
energy-rich links between the atoms, called chemical bonds. If some of
these bonds are then broken so that the large glucose molecule is split
into smaller molecules of water and carbon dioxide, the energy that was
once used to hold the whole glucose molecule together is then released.
So the body plunders the molecules by breaking them up and steals their
energy to keep itself going. It could do nothing unless it was
constantly fuelled by this energy. It would grind to a halt, lifeless
and inert. You could not move a muscle or think a thought without this
constant molecular smashing that your body does and without the energy
from the broken molecules being transported by yet other molecules to
the places where it is needed. The bird could not build its nest and we
could not watch it or wonder why it did so. Molecules have gone far,
considering that they are "nothing but" molecules.
There! The
few minutes of hallucination are up. I'll untie you now and you can go
back to thinking that scientific explanations diminish and belittle
everything they touch. There is just one thing, though. The prince in
C. S. Lewis's story had rather an unusual sort of hallucination. For
those few minutes each day, he suffered from the vivid delusion that
there was something beyond the dark underground kingdom in which he had
been living. He really believed- poor demented soul-that there was
something called sunlight and a place where the sky was blue and there
could be a fresh breeze on his cheek. But that was only for a few
minutes. And then it passed.
MARIAN STAMP DAWKINS has
a lifelong interest in what the worlds of different animals are
like-that is, not just in what they can see, hear, or smell, but in
what they know about their worlds and, above all, in whether they are conscious of
what they are doing. At the same time, she firmly believes that answers
to these issues should come not from anthropomorphism but from
scientific research and, in particular, from studies of animal
behaviour.
Much of her own research has been concerned with animal
welfare and, in particular, with the problem of whether animals can
experience "suffering." She has published extensively in this field, including the book Animal Suffering: The Science of Animal Welfare. She has also written Unravelling Animal Behaviour and An Introduction to Animal Behaviour (with Aubrey Manning). Most recently, she published Through Our Eyes Only? The Search for Animal Consciousness. Her current research concerns the evolution of animal signals, how birds recognise each other as individuals, and why the fish on coral reefs are so brightly coloured.
She holds a university research lectureship in the Department of
Zoology at Oxford University and is a fellow of Somerville College.